政大機構典藏-National Chengchi University Institutional Repository(NCCUR):Item 140.119/106433
English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  全文笔数/总笔数 : 109952/140903 (78%)
造访人次 : 46049299      在线人数 : 554
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
搜寻范围 查询小技巧:
  • 您可在西文检索词汇前后加上"双引号",以获取较精准的检索结果
  • 若欲以作者姓名搜寻,建议至进阶搜寻限定作者字段,可获得较完整数据
  • 进阶搜寻
    政大機構典藏 > 理學院 > 心理學系 > 學位論文 >  Item 140.119/106433


    请使用永久网址来引用或连结此文件: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/106433


    题名: 從眼動證據探索高功能自閉症類群障礙兒童的生物性運動知覺歷程
    An Eye-Tracking Study on Biological Motion Perception in Children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder
    作者: 林宛柔
    Lin, Wan-Jou
    贡献者: 姜忠信
    蔡介立

    Chiang, Chung-Hsin
    Tsai, Jie-Li

    林宛柔
    Lin, Wan-Jou
    关键词: 高功能自閉症類群障礙
    生物性運動知覺
    眼動追蹤技術
    症狀嚴重度
    適應行為
    智能優異
    High-Functioning Autism Spectrum disorder
    Biological motion perception
    Eye-tracking technique
    Severity symptom
    Adaptive function
    Intellectually gifted
    日期: 2016
    上传时间: 2017-02-08 16:41:17 (UTC+8)
    摘要: 研究目的:本研究旨在探究高功能自閉症類群障礙(High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder, HFASD)兒童基本的運動知覺、動物生物性運動知覺、與人類生物性運動知覺之偏好注視表現與注意力時間歷程,逐步澄清HFASD兒童的生物性運動知覺表現與社會缺損的關聯性。此外,針對自閉症類群障礙的異質性,進一步探討HFASD兒童具智能優異特質者之生物性運動知覺表現。
    研究方法:本研究共招募50名7至10歲之HFASD兒童與25名配對生理年齡與智力之TD兒童。本研究採偏好注視作業,以光點呈現三種運動刺激對比的視覺配對情境:(1)基本運動知覺:物體運動配對散亂運動;(2)動物生物性運動知覺:動物運動配對物體運動;(3)人類生物性運動知覺:人類運動配對動物運動。透過眼動追蹤技術測量受試兒童觀看各運動刺激之凝視時間比例與時間歷程之凝視可能性。研究亦涵蓋智力、症狀嚴重度、及適應行為評估。
    研究結果:基本運動知覺方面,TD兒童與HFASD 兒童皆對散亂運動具偏好反應。生物性運動知覺方面,整體來說,TD與HFASD兒童皆較偏好動物與人類運動。時間歷程分析顯示,HFASD與TD兒童在相同的時間窗格對動物與人類運動產生偏好興趣。此外,在動物生物性運動知覺中,於時間歷程早期階段,HFASD兒童偏好動物運動之程度低於TD兒童,且偏好動物運動的程度與社會溝通缺損具相關性,於晚期階段,兩組偏好動物運動的程度無顯著差異。在人類生物性運動知覺中,於時間歷程早期階段,HFASD兒童偏好人類運動的程度與TD兒童相當,隨時間遞增,TD兒童維持對人類運動的偏好興趣,然而,HFASD兒童對人類運動的偏好興趣則逐步遞減,至晚期階段,HFASD兒童偏好人類運動的程度顯著低於TD兒童,且與自閉症狀具相關性。考量本研究HFASD兒童樣本之異質性,結果指出智能優異的HFASD兒童對動物生物性運動的偏好程度與TD兒童相當,但在人類生物性運動知覺中,智能優異組與非智能優異組皆隨時間遞增對人類運動的偏好興趣則逐步遞減。
    總結:研究結果指出,HFASD兒童對生物性運動的偏好興趣及產生偏好的速度與TD兒童無明顯差異。但HFASD兒童對人類生物性運動偏好興趣的持續度較TD兒童低落,且不因認知優勢具補償作用。整體而言,顯示HFASD兒童的生物性運動知覺表現型態在反映其社會缺損上具有參考價值。本研究也進一步探討臨床應用、研究限制與未來的研究方向。
    Purposes: This study investigated the preferential attention and attentional processing on the basic motion perception, animal biological motion perception, and human biological motion perception in children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD). It aimed to explore the processing of biological motion perception in children with HFASD and the association to social deficit. Moreover, concerning the heterogeneity within ASD, the study examined the biological motion perception in intellectually gifted children within HFASD.
    Method: Fifty children aged 7 to 10 years with HFASD and twenty-five children with typically development(TD)matched on age and IQ were recruited. The current study utilized preferential looking paradigm and motion point-light displays, demonstrating three visual comparison: (1) basic motion perception: object motion paired with scrambled motion; (2) animal biological motion perception: animal motion paired with object motion; (3) human biological motion: human motion paired with animal motion. Eye-tracking techniques were applied to measure proportion of dwell time and time course of fixation probability on each motion stimuli. Cognitive function, symptom severity, and adaptive function were also measured.
    Result: In basic motion perception, children with TD and HFASD preferentially attend toward scrambled motion. In biological motion perception, generally, children with TD and HFASD preferentially attend toward animal motion and human motion. Time-course analysis revealed that children with TD and HFASD attended toward animal motion and human motion at the same time window. Moreover, in animal biological motion perception, children with HFASD showed lower preferential interest in animal motion than children with TD at the early stage of time course, and there were no significnant difference between groups at the late stage of time course. The results also found that the level of preference for animal motion was associated with the severity of social communication. In human biological motion perception, children with TD and HFASD showed similar level of preferential interest in human motion at the early stage of time course; afterthat, children with TD matained preferential interest in human motion across time, but children with HFASD reduced preferential interest in human motion across time. At the late stage of time course, children with HFASD showed significantly lower level preferential interest in human motion than children with TD, and it was associated to symptom severity. Concerning the heterogeneity within the study sample of HFASD, results indicated that there was no difference on the level of preferential interest in animal motion between intellectually gifted HFASD (IG-HFASD) and TD group; however, in human biological motion perception, IG-HFASD and nonIG-HFASD group reduced preferential interest in human motion across time course compared to TD group.
    Conclusion: Findings suggested that children with HFASD exhibit the equivalent preferential interest and speed of attending to biological motion as did TD children. However, children with HFASD reduced referential interest in human biological motion across time course compared to TD children, and not influenced by the intellectually gifted advantage with compensation. The results implicated that biological motion perception may play an important role to understanding the social deficit in children with ASD. Theoretical and clinical implications of the study were discussed.
    參考文獻: 陳心怡(1999)。「簡式」魏氏兒童智力量表之建立研究─四個分測驗之組合。中國測驗學會測驗年刊,46,13-32。
    陳榮華、陳心怡(1997)。魏氏兒童智力量表第三版(中文版)。台北:中國行為科學社。
    張正芬、陳美芳(2007)。亞斯柏格資優生適性教育方案第一年成果簡介。載於國科會學科教育主辦之「96年度數理特殊教育專題研究計畫」成果討論會論文集。
    劉昱志、劉士愷、商志雍、林健禾、杜長齡、高淑芬(2006)。注意力缺陷過動症中文版 Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, Version IV (SNAP-IV) 量表之常模及信效度,臺灣精神醫學,20(4),290-304。
    鄒小蘭、盧台華(2001)。身心障礙資優生優弱勢分析結果之探究。特殊教育學報,33,57-92。
    Abrahamson, S.J., Enticott, P.G., & Tonge, B.J. (2010). High-functioning pervasive developmental disorders in adults. Medical Journal of Australia, 192(1), 44-48.
    Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267-278.
    American Psychiatric Association (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 1st ed.). Washington DC: Author.
    American Psychiatric Association (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 2ed ed.). Washington DC: Author.
    American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed.). Washington DC: Author.
    American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders -Text Revision ( 4th ed-TR.). Washington DC: Author.
    American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-Fifth edition ( 5th ed.). Washington DC: Author.
    Annaz, D., Campbell, R., Coleman, M., Mline, E., & Swettenham, J. (2012). Young children with autism spectrum disorder do not preferentially attend to biological motion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(3), 401-408.
    Annaz, D., Remington, A., Milne, E., Coleman, M., Campbell, R., Thomas, M. SC, & Swettenham, J. (2010). Development of motion processing in children with autism. Developmental science, 13(6), 826-838.
    Atkinson, A. P, Dittrich, W. H, Gemmell, A. J, & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33(6), 717-746.
    Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S.C.R. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(3), 355-364.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a" language of the eyes"? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4(3), 311-331.
    Baron-Cohen, Simon, Wheelwright, Sally, Lawson, John, Griffin, Richard, Ashwin, Chris, Billington, Jac, & Chakrabarti, Bhismadev. (2005). Empathizing and systemizing in autism spectrum conditions. In F. Volkmar, A. Klin & R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 628-639).
    Bates, E. (1979). On the evolution and development of symbols. In E. Bates (Ed.), The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy (pp. 1-32). New York: Academic Press.
    Bender, L. (1969). A longitudinal study of schizophrenic children with autism. Psychiatric Services, 20(8), 230-237.
    Bidet-Ildei, C., Kitromilides, E., Orliaguet, J., Pavlova, M., & Gentaz, E. (2013). Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: Contribution of translational displacement. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 113.
    Bishop, S. L., Hus, V., Duncan, A., Huerta, M., Gotham, K., Pickles, A., ... & Lord, C. (2013). Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(6), 1287-1297.
    Bishop, S. L., Richler, J., & Lord, C. (2006). Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 12(4-5), 247-267.
    Blake, R., Turner, L.M., Smoski, M.J., Pozodol, S.L., & Stone, W.L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14(2), 151-157.
    Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 47-73.
    Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Ostry, D., & Evans, A. (1996). Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16(11), 3737-3744.
    Bowler, D. M. (1992). "Theory of Mind" in Asperger`s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877-893.
    Bushnell, E. W., & Boudreau, J. P. (1993). Motor development and the mind: the potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development. Child Development, 64(4), 1005-1021.
    Carter, A. S., Volkmor, F., Sparrow, S. S., Wang, J. J., Lord, C., Dawson, G., . . . Schopler, E. (1998). The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supplementary norms for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(4), 287-302.
    Cash, A. B. (1999). A profile of gifted individuals with autism: The twice‐exceptional learner. Roeper Review, 22(1), 22-27.
    Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12(3), 314-325.
    Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 195-203.
    Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 178-185.
    Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231-239.
    Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The visual analysis of emotional actions. Social Neuroscience, 1(1), 63-74.
    Churchill, D.W. (1972). The relation of infantile autism and early childhood schizophrenia to developmental language disorders of childhood. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(2), 182-197.
    Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: a review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48(1), 79-93.
    Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2005). Social Responsiveness Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
    Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E., & Palmer, J. (2002). The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34(4), 613-617.
    Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Ring, R. H. (2012). Social attention: a possible early indicator of efficacy in autism clinical trials. Journal of Neurodevlopmental Disorder, 4(11), 1-12.
    Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development, 73(3), 700-717.
    Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271-283.
    Dawson, G., Webb, S.J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403-424.
    DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers` voices. Science, 208(4448), 1174-1176.
    Dittrich, W. H. (1993). Action categories and the perception of biological motion. Percption, 22(1), 15-22.
    Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). Infancy and autism: Progress, prospects, and challenges. Progress in Brain Research, 164(1), 355-383.
    Engel, A. K., Fries, P., & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Review Neuroscience, 2(10), 704-716.
    Fischer, J., Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., & Kanwisher, N. (2014). Unimpaired attentional disengagement and social orienting in children with autism. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 214-223.
    Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248-257.
    Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218(4571), 486-487.
    Franchini, M., de Wilde, H. W., Glaser, B., Gentaz, E., Eliez, S., & Schaer, M. (2016). Brief report: A preference for biological motion predicts a reduction in symptom severity 1 year later in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7(143), 1-8.
    Freitag, C., Konrad, C., Häberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., . . . Krick, C. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1480-1494.
    Frith, C. D, & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds--a biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692-1695.
    Frith, C. D, & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531-534.
    Gillberg, C. L. (1992). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1991. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 813-842.
    Gillham, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. . (2000). Toward a developmental operational definition of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), 269-278.
    Gobbini, M, Koralek, A, Bryan, R, Montgomery, K, & Haxby, J. (2007). Two takes on the social brain: a comparison of theory of mind tasks. Jouranl of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1803-1814.
    Goldstein, G., Allen, D.N., Minshew, N. J., Williams, D. L., Volkmar, F., Klin, A., & Schultz, R.T. (2008). The structure of intelligence in children and adults with high functioning autism. Neuropsychology, 22(3), 301.
    Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 213-225.
    Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711-720.
    Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1997). Does teaching theory of mind have an effect on the ability to develop conversation in children with autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 519-537.
    Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1979). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198(4319), 853-855.
    Han, Z., Bi, Y., Chen, J., Chen, Q., He, Y., & Caramazza, A. (2013). Distinct Regions of Right Temporal Cortex Are Associated with Biological and Human–Agent Motion: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Neuropsychological Evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(39), 15442-15453.
    Happé, F. G., & Frith, U. (1996). Theory of mind and social impairment in children with conduct disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14(4), 385-398.
    Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218-1220.
    Heron-Delaney, M., Wirth, S., & Pascalis, O. (2011). Infants` knowledge of their own species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1753-1763.
    Herrington, J. D., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. J., Singh, K. D., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., & Williams, S.CR. (2007). The role of MT+/V5 during biological motion perception in Asperger Syndrome: An fMRI study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(1), 14-27.
    Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212-229.
    Hood, B.M., Willen, J.D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult`s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131-134.
    Houston‐Price, C., & Nakai, S. (2004). Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures. Infant and Child Development, 13(4), 341-348.
    Howard, R.J., Brammer, M., Wright, I., Woodruff, P.W., Bullmore, E.T., & Zeki, S. (1996). A direct demonstration of functional specialization within motion-related visual and auditory cortex of the human brain. Current Biology, 6(8), 1015-1019.
    Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(1), 3-13.
    Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children with autism to mind-read. New York: Wiley.
    Huber, D. H. (2007). Clinical presentation of autism spectrum disorders in intellectually gifted students (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertation and Theses Database. (UMI No. 3301714)
    Hubert, B, Wicker, B, Moore, Derek G, Monfardini, E, Duverger, H, Da Fonseca, D, & Deruelle, C. (2007). Brief report: recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1386-1392.
    Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(2), 201-211.
    Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: Viewpoint-dependent effects. Perception, 35(7), 911-920.
    Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-olds with autism spectrum disorder. Archives General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946-954.
    Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism spectrum: The role of development. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 471-473.
    Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427-431.
    Joseph, RM, & Tanaka, J. (2002). Holistic and part‐based face recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 529-542.
    Kaiser, M. D., Delmolino, L., Tanaka, J. W., & Shiffrar, M. (2010). Comparison of visual sensitivity to human and object motion in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 3(4), 191-195.
    Kaiser, M. D, Shiffrar, M., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2012). Socially tuned: brain responses differentiating human and animal motion. Social Neuroscience, 7(3), 301-310.
    Kaiser, M. D., Hudac, C. M., Shultz, S., Lee, S. M., Cheung, C., Berken, A. M., . . . Voos, A. C. (2010). Neural signatures of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(49), 21223-21228.
    Kaiser, M. D., & Shiffrar, M. (2012). Variability in the visual perception of human motion as a function of the observer’s autistic traits. In K. Johnson & M. D. Kaiser (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 159-178). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
    Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217-250.
    Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher‐functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 831-846.
    Klin, A. (2006). Autism and Asperger syndrome: an overview. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 28(1), 3-11.
    Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2008). Altered face scanning and impared recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. Development Science, 11(1), 40-46.
    Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmor, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 358(1430), 345-360.
    Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmor, F., & Cohen, D. (2002a). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic. Archives General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809-816.
    Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmor, F., & Cohen, D. (2002b). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. Archives General Psychiatry, 159(6), 895-908.
    Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism fail to orient towards human biological motion but attend instead to non-social, physical contingencies. Nature, 459(7244), 257-261.
    Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Volkmar, F. R. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 748-759.
    Kozlowski, L. T, & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21(6), 575-580.
    Landa, R. (2007). Early communication development and intervention for children with autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(1), 16-25.
    Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: a review of research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 562.
    Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Scales (ADOS). LA: Western Psychological Services.
    Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 210-220.
    MacDonald, R., Green, G., Mansfield, R., Geckeler, A., Gardenier, N., Anderson, J., ... & Sanchez, J. (2007). Stereotypy in young children with autism and typically developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(3), 266-277.
    Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Imitation and other minds: The “like me” hypothesis. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (pp. 55-77). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Moore, D. G. (2011). Understanding of human motion, form and levels of meaning: evidence from the perception of human point-light displays by infants and people with autism. In V. Slaughter & C. A. Brownell (Eds.), Early development of body representations (pp. 122-145). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Anderson, M. (1995). Person perception: Does it involve IQ-independent perceptual processing? Intelligence, 20(1), 65-86.
    Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 15(4), 401-423.
    Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 63-82.
    Mundy, P., & Neal, A. R. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 139-168.
    Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). The theory of mind and joint-attention deficits in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 181-203). New York: Oxford University Press.
    New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(42), 16598-16603.
    Nicpon, M. F., Doobay, A. F., & Assouline, S. G. (2010). Parent, teacher, and self perceptions of psychosocial functioning in intellectually gifted children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1028-1038.
    Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 247-257.
    Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: a new apprach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(4), 415-433.
    Parron, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 12(3), 261-274.
    Pelphrey, K. A, Morris, J. P, Michelich, C. R, Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an fMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 15(12), 1866-1876.
    Pelphrey, K. A., Shultz, S., Hudac, C. M., & Vander Wyk, B. C. (2011). Research review: constraining heterogeneity: the social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 631-644.
    Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Geier, J. D., & Freeman, N. L. (2009). Brief report: The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in young children with autism spectrum disorders at different cognitive levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 1066-1078.
    Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(1), 101-109.
    Pierce, K., Marinero, S., Hazin, R., McKenna, B., Barnes, C. C., & Malige, A. (2016). Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity. Biological Psychiatry, 79(8), 657-666.
    Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of human and animal motion in point-light displays. Social Neuroscience, 4(4), 332-346.
    Poizner, H., Bellugi, U., & Lutes-Driscoll, V. (1981). Perception of American sign language in dynamic point-light displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(2), 430.
    Pollick, F. E, Kay, J. W, Heim, K., & Stringer, R. (2005). Gender recognition from point-light walkers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1247-1265.
    Pollick, F. E, Paterson, H. M, Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82(2), 51-61.
    Powell, G., Wass, S. V., Erichsen, J. T., & Leekam, S. R. (2016). First evidence of the feasibility of gaze-contingent attention training for school children with autism. Autism, 20(8), 927-937.
    Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C, & McCarthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(6), 2188-2199.
    R, Development CoreTeam. (2010). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Wien: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (Computer software).
    Rice, K., Moriuchi, JM, Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 238-248.
    Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(4), 585-615.
    Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia reconsidered. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(3), 315-337.
    Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire-WPS. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    Rutter, M., LeCouteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R-WPS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1987). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Concepts and diagnostic issues. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), 159-186.
    Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 584-592.
    Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(1), 170-175.
    Shic, F., Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Scassellati, B., & Chawarska, K. (2011). Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1380, 246-254.
    Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., & Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature neuroscience, 6(12), 1317-1322.
    Sigman, M., Yirmiya, N., & Capps, L. (1995). Social and cognitive understanding in high-functioning children with autism. New York: Plenum.
    Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105(2), 809-813.
    South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W.M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 145-158.
    Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. A., & Balla, D. A. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Expanded Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
    Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. A., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (2th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
    Spezio, M.L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R.S.E, & Piven, J. (2007). Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 929-939.
    Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S, Charman, T, Cox, A, Baird, G, Drew, A_, . . . Wheelwright, S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 747-753.
    Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 311-315.
    Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R.M. (2005). How language facil- itates the acquisition of false belief understanding in children with autism. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 298-318). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
    Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675-691.
    Trepagnier, C., Sebrechts, MM, & Peterson, R. (2002). Atypical face gaze in autism. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 5(3), 213-217.
    Troje, N. F. (2008). Biological motion perception. In: Allan Basbaum et al (Eds.), The Senses: A Comprehensive References (pp. 231-238). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
    Troje, N. F. (2013). What is biological motion? Definition, stimuli and paradigms. In D. Rutherford & V. A. Kuhlmeier (Eds.), Social Perception: Detection and Interpretation of Animacy, Agency, and Intention (pp. 13-36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Vecera, S.P., & Johnson, M.H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition, 2(1), 59-87.
    Wang, L. H., Chien, S. H., Hu, S. F., Chen, T. Y., & Chen, H. S. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorders are less proficient in action identification and lacking preference for upright point-light biological motion displays. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11(1), 63-76.
    Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157-162.
    Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1988). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283-307.
    Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2), 143-152.
    描述: 碩士
    國立政治大學
    心理學系
    102752004
    資料來源: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G1027520041
    数据类型: thesis
    显示于类别:[心理學系] 學位論文

    文件中的档案:

    档案 大小格式浏览次数
    004101.pdf3696KbAdobe PDF2211检视/开启


    在政大典藏中所有的数据项都受到原著作权保护.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - 回馈