政大機構典藏-National Chengchi University Institutional Repository(NCCUR):Item 140.119/124864
English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 109952/140903 (78%)
Visitors : 46032147      Online Users : 713
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/124864


    Title: 高功能自閉症類群障礙兒童心智理論與執行功能之探究:與症狀嚴重度及適應行為之關聯性
    Exploration of Theory of Mind and Executive Function in Children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder : Relationship with Symptom Severity and Adaptive Function
    Authors: 陳昱伶
    Chen, Yu-Ling
    Contributors: 姜忠信
    Chiang, Chung-Hsin
    陳昱伶
    Chen, Yu-Ling
    Keywords: 高功能自閉症類群障礙症
    心智理論
    執行功能
    自閉症狀
    適應功能
    注意力不足過動症特徵
    High-functioning autism spectrum disorder
    Theory of mind
    Executive function
    Autistic symptom
    Adaptive functioning
    ADHD trait
    Date: 2019
    Issue Date: 2019-08-07 16:34:25 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 研究目的:本研究主要目的為探究高功能自閉症類群障礙(High-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder, HFASD)兒童心智理論與執行功能之關聯性,並探索心智理論與執行功能表現能否反映其自閉症狀嚴重度及適應行為差異。此外,呼應自閉症類群障礙的異質性,嘗試以心智理論與執行功能表現描繪具注意力不足過動症特徵HFASD兒童之特性。
    研究方法:本研究共招募生理年齡7-10歲的66名HFASD兒童與40名一般發展兒童,接受一項心智理論作業(含諷刺及隱喻分測驗)與三項執行功能作業(強勢反應抑制、抑制分心物干擾及心智彈性),並評估其智力、自閉症症狀、注意力不足過動症症狀與適應行為表現。
    研究結果:相關性分析結果顯示,心智理論與執行功能作業之間顯著的相關性普遍存在於HFASD組,一般發展組則僅有強勢抑制反應時間與心智理論作業有顯著的負相關。缺損型態結果指出近四成HFASD兒童的心智理論與執行功能皆無缺損,僅心智理論缺損以及僅執行功能缺損的人數比例則相當;此外,具注意力不足過動症特徵的HFASD兒童更容易發生心智理論的缺損。行為症狀方面,不同缺損型態略能反映HFASD兒童的自閉症狀嚴重度,且心智理論與執行功能皆呈現缺損的HFASD兒童有更嚴重的溝通症狀;然而,此缺損型態無法反映出HFASD兒童的適應行為問題。
    總結:學齡HFASD兒童的心智理論與執行功能之間仍具有顯著的相關性,且心智理論與執行功能缺損型態可反映HFASD兒童部分的自閉症狀嚴重度,凸顯同時考量執行功能與心智理論的重要性。
    Purposes: The purpose of the study was to explore the relationship of the Theory of Mind (ToM) and Executive Finction (EF) in children with High-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD), and explore whether the performance of ToM and EF can reflect severity of autistic symptom and adaptive function. In addition, concerning the heterogeneity within ASD, the study was also attempted to use ToM and EF to identify ADHD trait within HFASD.
    Methods: Sixty-six HFASD children and fourty typically development (TD) children were recruited, and all children were aged 7-10 years. Participants were assessed on a battery of tasks measuring ToM (including irony and metaphor) and EF (including inhibition control and cognitive flexibility). IQ, autism severity, ADHD trait and adaptive function were also measured.
    Results: Performance on ToM and EF tasks was significantly related to each other in the HFASD group, while there was only a significant negative correlation between ToM and reaction time of prepotent inhibition in the TD group. Examination of the pattern of EF-ToM impairments in the HFASD group, indicating nearly 40% children with HFASD demonstrated intact EF and ToM, while the propotions of impaired ToM and impaired EF were equivalent; furthermore, the children with HFASD combining with ADHD trait proned to demonstrate impaired ToM. The pattern of EF-ToM impairments can explain the severity of autistic symptom. HFASD children who demonstrated impaired EF and ToM also showed more symptoms in communication. However, the pattern of EF-ToM impairments cannot explain the adaptive function.
    Conclusions: A significant relationship were revealed between ToM and EF in the shool-aged children with HFASD. Autism severity can be partially explained by the pattern of EF-ToM impairments. The results indicated the importance roles of both the EF and ToM to explain the heterogeneity of HFASD.
    Reference: 李竹芳(2009)。高功能泛自閉症學童執行功能與侷限重複症狀之初探。碩士論文,私立中原大學心理學研究所,未出版,桃園縣。
    林怡安(2017)。高功能自閉症類群障礙兒童的執行功能之探究:與症狀嚴重度、適應行為及行為問題表現之關聯性。碩士論文,國立政治大學心理學研究所臨床心理學組,未出版,台北市。
    許君翎(2007)。注意力缺陷/過動疾患兒童與高功能自閉症兒童在心智理論能力上的差異。碩士論文,輔仁大學心理學研究所臨床心理學組,未出版,台北市。
    陳心怡(1999)。「簡式」魏氏兒童智力量表之建立—四個分測驗之組合。測驗年刊,46(2),13-32。
    陳心怡、廖永堃、陳榮華、朱建軍(2011)。臺灣兒童於 1997 至 2007 年智能進步狀況分析:弗林效應之探討。教育科學研究期刊,56(1),167-191。
    陳榮華(1997)。魏氏兒童智力量表第三版(中文版)指導手冊。台北:中國行為科學社。
    陳榮華、陳心怡(2007)。魏氏兒童智力量表第四版(中文版)技術和解釋手冊。台北:中國行為科學社。
    鄒啟蓉(2005)。學齡前幼兒 “錯誤信念” 發展研究。特殊教育研究學刊,(29),25-47。
    劉昱志、劉士愷、商志雍、林健禾、杜長齡、高淑芬(2006)。 注意力缺陷過動症中文版 Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, Version IV (SNAP-IV) 量表之常模及信效度。臺灣精神醫學,20(4),290-304。
    鄧宇捷(2017)。高功能自閉症類群障礙兒童心智理論能力表現之研究。碩士論文,國立政治大學心理學研究所臨床心理學組,未出版,台北市。
    Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: Children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(6), 1052-1063.
    Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short‐term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child development, 77(6), 1698-1716.
    American Psychiatric Association (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 1st ed.). Washington DC: Author.

    American Psychiatric Association (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 2ed ed.). Washington DC: Author.

    American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed.). Washington DC: Author.

    American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
    disorders -Text Revision ( 4th ed-TR.). Washington DC: Author.

    American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-Fifth edition ( 5th ed.). Washington DC: Author.
    Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(6), 248-254.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46.
    Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1994). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. In Most of the chapters in this book were presented in draft form at a workshop in Seattle, Apr 1991. Oxford University Press.
    Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. The Journal of general psychology, 39(1), 15-22.
    Begeer, S., Howlin, P., Hoddenbach, E., Clauser, C., Lindauer, R., Clifford, P., ... & Koot, H. M. (2015). Effects and moderators of a short theory of mind intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Autism Research, 8(6), 738-748.
    Bleuler, E. (1911). Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Handbuch der psychiatrie.
    Broadbent, J., & Stokes, M. A. (2013). Removal of negative feedback enhances WCST performance for individuals with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(6), 785-792.
    Brunsdon, V. E. (2016). Exploring the fractionation of autism spectrum disorder at the cognitive level (Doctoral dissertation, King`s College London).
    Brunsdon, V. E., & Happé, F. (2014). Exploring the ‘fractionation’of autism at the cognitive level. Autism, 18(1), 17-30.
    Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., ... & Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16(5), 378-384.
    Cantio, C., Jepsen, J. R. M., Madsen, G. F., Bilenberg, N., & White, S. J. (2016). Exploring ‘The autisms’ at a cognitive level. Autism Research, 9(12), 1328-1339.
    Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental psychology, 40(6), 1105.
    Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children`s theory of mind. Child development, 72(4), 1032-1053.
    Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73-92.
    Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of experimental child psychology, 87(4), 299-319.
    Chang, F., & Burns, B. M. (2005). Attention in preschoolers: Associations with effortful control and motivation. Child Development, 76(1), 247-263.
    Craig, F., Lamanna, A. L., Margari, F., Matera, E., Simone, M., & Margari, L. (2015). Overlap between autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: searching for distinctive/common clinical features. Autism research, 8(3), 328-337.
    Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “Do as I say, not as I do”. Developmental psychobiology, 29(4), 315-334.
    Dichter, G. S., Radonovich, K. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Lam, K. S., Holtzclaw, T. N., & Bodfish, J. W. (2010). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 448-456.
    Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child development, 79(1), 126-138.
    Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. (1983). Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive psychology, 15(1), 95-120.
    Flynn, J. R. (2006). Tethering the elephant: Capital cases, IQ, and the Flynn effect. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 12(2), 170.
    Flynn, E., O`Malley, C., & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7(1), 103-115.
    Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma (Vol. 1989). Blackwell Scientific Publications: Oxford.
    Frith, U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2073-2092.
    Frith, U., Happé, F., & Siddons, F. (1994). Autism and theory of mind in everyday life. Social development, 3(2), 108-124.
    Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive development, 10(4), 483-527.
    Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Sirian, L., Black, D. O., & Wagner, A. E. (2002). Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 241-248.
    Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235-238.
    Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of experimental psychology, 38(4), 404.
    Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child development, 70(4), 817-832.
    Grzadzinski, R., Huerta, M., & Lord, C. (2013). DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders (ASDs): an opportunity for identifying ASD subtypes. Molecular autism, 4(1), 12.
    Hala, S., & Russell, J. (2001). Executive control within strategic deception: a window on early cognitive development? Journal of experimental child psychology, 80(2), 112-141.
    Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism, 9(2), 157-178.
    Happé, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101-119.
    Happé, F. G. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(6), 216-222.
    Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and cognition, 61(1), 25-39.
    Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature neuroscience, 9(10), 1218-1220.
    Hill, E. L. (2004a). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental review, 24(2), 189-233.
    Hill, E. L. (2004b). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(1), 26-32.
    Hughes, C. (1998a). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233-253.
    Hughes, C. (1998b). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers` strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind?. Developmental psychology, 34(6), 1326.
    Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair?. Developmental neuropsychology, 28(2), 645-668.
    Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental psychology, 43(6), 1447.
    Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children`s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental psychology, 29(3), 498.
    Hughes, C., Soares-Boucaud, I., Hochmann, J., & Frith, U. (1997). Social behaviour in pervasive developmental disorders: Effects of informant, group and “theory-of-mind”. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 6(4), 191-198.
    Hutchins, T. L., Prelock, P. A., Morris, H., Benner, J., LaVigne, T., & Hoza, B. (2016). Explicit vs. applied theory of mind competence: A comparison of typically developing males, males with ASD, and males with ADHD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 21, 94-108.
    Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental psychology, 32(1), 70.
    Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. E. L. E. N. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and psychopathology, 16(1), 137-155.
    Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127(8), 1811-1821.
    Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 38(6), 1161-1165.
    Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous child, 2(3), 217-250.
    Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension‐switching task. Developmental Science, 6(5), 449-467.
    Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(7), 317-324.
    Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2003). Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: Helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child development, 74(6), 1823-1839.
    Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY-II: Clinical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
    Kouklari, E. C., Tsermentseli, S., & Auyeung, B. (2018). Executive function predicts theory of mind but not social verbal communication in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in developmental disabilities, 76, 12-24.
    Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., Chakrabarti B., MRC AIMS Consortium & Baron-Cohen S. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. Plos one, 6(6), e20835.
    Lord, C., & Risi, S. (1998). Frameworks and methods in diagnosing autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4(2), 90-96.
    Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20(1), 13-32.
    McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 34(4), 563-578.
    Morton, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Why we need cognition: Cause and developmental disorder. Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler, 263.
    Moses, L. J. (2001). Executive Accounts of Theory‐of‐Mind Development. Child development, 72(3), 688-690.
    Moses, L. J., & Tahiroglu, D. E. N. I. Z. (2010). Clarifying the relation between executive function and children’s theories of mind. Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions, 218-233.
    Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383-399.
    Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 29(2), 171-177.
    Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high‐functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081-1105.
    Panerai, S., Tasca, D., Ferri, R., Genitori D’Arrigo, V., & Elia, M. (2014). Executive functions and adaptive behaviour in autism spectrum disorders with and without intellectual disability. Psychiatry journal, 2014.
    Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: clues to developmental primacy. Developmental psychology, 43(4), 974.
    Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental psychology, 46(2), 530.
    Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 37(1), 51-87.
    Perra, O., Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Fraser, L., Benzie, H., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Imitation and ‘theory of mind’competencies in discrimination of autism from other neurodevelopmental disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 456-468.
    Peterson, C. C. (2005). Mind and body: Concepts of human cognition, physiology and false belief in children with autism or typical development. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 35(4), 487-497.
    Peterson, C. C., Garnett, M., Kelly, A., & Attwood, T. (2009). Everyday social and conversation applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with autism-spectrum disorders or typical development. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 18(2), 105-115.
    Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory‐of‐mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child development, 83(2), 469-485.
    Perner, J. (1998). The meta-intentional nature of executive functions and theory of mind. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes (pp. 270–283). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(9), 337-344.
    Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-year-old children. Journal of experimental child psychology, 39(3), 437-471.
    Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S., & Mozes, T. (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia research, 42(2), 145-155.
    Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and brain sciences, 1(4), 515-526.
    Prior, M., Eisenmajer, R., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Gould, J., Ong, B., & Dowe, D. (1998). Are there subgroups within the autistic spectrum? A cluster analysis of a group of children with autistic spectrum disorders. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(6), 893-902.
    Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Brief report: Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 20(4), 581-590.
    Putko, A. (2009). Links between theory of mind and executive function: towards a more comprehensive model. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 40(4), 156-162.
    Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and cognition, 71(3), 362-368.
    Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1985). Temperament and the development of self-regulation. In The neuropsychology of individual differences (pp. 93-123). Springer US.
    Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029-1040.
    Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D. (2010). The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with autism. Autism, 14(1), 29-46.
    Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring about an adequate theory of mind. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 256–304). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ. The Social Communication Questionnaire. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    Semrud-Clikeman, M., Fine, J. G., & Bledsoe, J. (2014). Comparison among children with children with autism spectrum disorder, nonverbal learning disorder and typically developing children on measures of executive functioning. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 44(2), 331-342.
    Shu, B. C., Lung, F. W., Tien, A. Y., & Chen, B. C. (2001). Executive function deficits in non-retarded autistic children. Autism, 5(2), 165-174.
    Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1996). Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 26(4), 389-406.
    South, M., Ozonoff, S., & Mcmahon, W. M. (2007). The relationship between executive functioning, central coherence, and repetitive behaviors in the high-functioning autism spectrum. Autism, 11(5), 437-451.
    Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland adaptive behavior scales: Expanded form. American Guidance Service.
    Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005). Vineland II: A revision of the vineland adaptive behavior scales: I. survey/caregiver form. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
    Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 395.
    Swanson, J. M., Nolan, W., & Pelham, W. E. (1982). The SNAP rating scale. Resources in Education.
    Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Exploring the relationship between theory of mind and social-communicative functioning in children with autism. In Repacholi B. & Slaughter V. (Eds.), Individual differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development (pp. 197-212). New York: Psychology Press.
    Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current directions in psychological science, 16(6), 311-315.
    Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). A second look at second-order belief attribution in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 577-586.
    Tager-Flusberg, H. & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59-89.
    Taurines, R., Schwenck, C., Westerwald, E., Sachse, M., Siniatchkin, M., & Freitag, C. (2012). ADHD and autism: differential diagnosis or overlapping traits? A selective review. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 4(3), 115-139.
    Tsai, L. Y. (1992). Diagnostic issues in high-functioning autism. In High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 11-40). Springer US.
    Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behaviour in autism: A review of psychological research. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(6), 839-849.
    Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35(3), 245-275.
    Welsh, M. C., & Pennington, B. F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Developmental neuropsychology, 4(3), 199-230.
    White, S. J. (2013). The triple I hypothesis: taking another (’s) perspective on executive dysfunction in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 114-121.
    White, S. J., Coniston, D., Rogers, R., & Frith, U. (2011). Developing the Frith-Happé animations: A quick and objective test of Theory of Mind for adults with autism. Autism Research, 4(2), 149-154.
    White, S., Hill, E., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2009). Revisiting the strange stories: Revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child development, 80(4), 1097-1117.
    WHO (World Health Organization). ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Geneva: WHO, 1993.
    Wimmer, H. (1989). Common-sense Mentalismus und Emotion: einige entwicklungspsychologische Implikationen. In Denken und Fühlen (pp. 56-66). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
    Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children`s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128.
    Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., Celano, M. J., Giedd, J. N., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders: reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism, 13(5), 523-538.
    Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive function: A problem-solving framework. Review of general psychology, 1(2), 198.
    Description: 碩士
    國立政治大學
    心理學系
    104752020
    Source URI: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G1047520201
    Data Type: thesis
    DOI: 10.6814/NCCU201900306
    Appears in Collections:[Department of Psychology] Theses

    Files in This Item:

    File SizeFormat
    020101.pdf3307KbAdobe PDF2191View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback