政大機構典藏-National Chengchi University Institutional Repository(NCCUR):Item 140.119/98620
English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 109948/140897 (78%)
Visitors : 46070816      Online Users : 775
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/98620


    Title: 以壓力反應特性、注意力偏誤、與睡眠監控行為探討不同 失眠病程發展之相關因素
    The Contributing Roles of Stress Reactivity, Attentional Bias, and Monitoring Behaviors in the Course of the Development of Insomnia
    Authors: 詹雅雯
    Jan, Ya Wen
    Contributors: 楊建銘
    Yang, Chien Ming
    詹雅雯
    Jan, Ya Wen
    Keywords: 慢性失眠
    失眠脆弱特質
    注意力偏誤
    壓力反應
    睡前激發狀態
    Chronic Insomnia
    Sleep Vulnerability
    Attention bias
    Stress reaction
    Presleep Arousal
    Date: 2016
    Issue Date: 2016-07-01 15:16:04 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 研究目的 失眠的過度激發是目前最廣為接受的失眠病因之一。無論在生理、認知、行為三不同層面上,多可觀察到失眠者有身心過度激發的狀態。根據失眠三因子模式,不同失眠病程階段,影響過度激發的背後成因有所差異。在慢性失眠部分,過
    去累積了相當多的實證研究證實其過度激發的現象,但尚未進入慢性病程前之過度激發相關機轉,仍有待研究進一步探討。本研究嘗試以橫斷式的研究方法,並依據過去失眠病因發展推導,選擇從壓力反應特性 (包含壓力操弄後的壓力反應強度和
    消退速度)、注意力偏誤 (包含警覺性注意力和注意力移除困難)、與睡前的睡眠監控行為三個面向切入,探討不同病程階段個體過度激發的背後機轉,並進一步檢驗上述之差異是否可用以預測在壓力操弄情境下,不同病程個體睡前的激發反應變化,藉此檢驗失眠病程發展之病因假說,並希望未來可據此發展有效之失眠防治與介入策略。
    研究方法 本研究共計招募受試者 58 人,年齡介於 24-48 歲,包含符合 ICSD-3 慢性失眠者 18 人,以及以壓力下失眠反應量表( Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test; FIRST)區分出急性失眠高危險組 19 人與低危險組 21 人。每位受試者皆需到睡眠實驗室進行兩階段的實驗,第一階段包含晤談評估、問卷填答、壓力反應的生理測量(以指溫與膚電為指標)、與包含威脅與睡眠刺激之點偵測注意力作業,之後需配戴腕錶與記錄睡眠日誌配合充足且規律作息一週後,再到睡眠實驗室進行第二階段的評量,包含睡前 2 小時、1 小時、關燈前生理指標 (指溫、膚電) 與腦波的測量和主觀身心激發狀態 (Pre-Sleep Arousal Scale, 簡稱 PSAS) 評量,於睡前填寫睡眠相關監測指標 (Sleep Associated Monitoring Index,簡稱 SAMI),並完成一晚的 PSG 測量以排除其他睡眠相關疾患。
    研究結果 首先,以單因子變異數分析比較不同組別間在壓力反應與回復和二因子變異數分析注意力警覺/移除困難指標的差異。在壓力生理反應表現上,慢性失眠組與高危險組在接受壓力操弄後的激發消退時間較低危險組來得長。在注意力層面,高危險組對壓力(威脅圖片) 刺激有顯著的警覺與移除困難注意力偏誤,慢性失眠組則是對睡眠刺激有顯著的移除困難注意力偏誤。在行為層面,慢性失眠組與高危險組睡前的注意力監控行為 (包含監測自身身體感覺訊息是否與入睡狀態不一致、鬧鐘
    時間、環境) 均顯著較低危險組來得多。再者,以皮爾森相關探討注意力偏誤與睡前激發反應之關聯性,結果顯示高危險組的注意力偏誤現象與睡前高頻腦波與主觀生理激發的降幅呈現顯著負相關; 而慢性失眠組的注意力偏誤指標卻與膚電、主觀認知激發的降幅呈顯著正相關。
    結論 本研究結果支持不同失眠病程背後的過度激發影響機制有所差異,生理層面較慢的激發消退能力與對壓力源的認知偏誤的前置因子,可能為急性失眠者易受日常壓力源誘發睡眠困擾之原因;而影響慢性失眠族群的持續因子主要在於其將睡眠視
    為壓力源的認知歷程與行為轉變。此外,研究更進一步發現兩組分別對於壓力與睡眠的注意力轉移困難,使其在覺察壓力後易持續表現出過度激發現象。本研究結果除支持失眠過度激發理論之外,更釐清不同階段失眠的認知歷程的機制,並彰顯不
    同失眠病程介入策略應有所差異,和急性失眠高危險族群及早介入預防之重要性。
    Introduction
    Hyperarousal has been recognized to be a major etiological factor of chronic insomnia. Cumulated research evidences have demonstrated that chronic insomnia patients are
    hyperaroused in somatic, cognitive, and behavioral aspects. According to Spielman’s 3P Model of Insomnia, there were different factors are involved at different points during the course of insomnia. However, there are seldom study to investigate the difference mechanism of hyperarousal in the course of the development of insomnia.
    The present study used cross-sectional design to compare the difference of good sleeper (low sleep vulnerability, LV), acute insomnia (high sleep vulnerability, HV),
    and chronic insomnia (CI) in stress reaction (eg. reactivity and recovery), attentional bias (eg. vigilance and disengagement), and sleep associated monitoring behaviors to investigate the underlying mechanism of hyperarousal. Furthermore, the study examined the correlation between attentional bias indices and subsequent pre-sleep arousal to investigate the impact of attentional bias on sleep in different groups.
    Method
    The present study recruited fifty-eight subjects, aged between 24-48. They included eighteen chronic insomniacs (CI) diagnosed ICSD-3, nineteen healthy individuals
    scoring high (HV) and twenty-one healthy individuals scoring low (LV) on the Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test (FIRST). All subjects visited sleep lab twice. During the first visit, the subjects filled in a package of questionnaires, and went through psychophysiological recording (including) of stress reaction, and a visual dot-probe task. They then were required to keep a sleep log and wear actigraphy at home for one-week to make sure they followed a regular sleep schedule. During the second visit, subjects went through a pre-sleep physiological recording (including peripheral temperature, skin conductance, and EEG) and filled in two questionnaires (Pre Sleep Arousal Scale [PSAS] and Sleep Associated Monitoring Index[SAMI]) at three time points and had a PSG recording to screen for sleep disorders.
    Result
    One-way ANOVAs were conducted to compare the differences of stress reaction/recovery among three groups. Two-way ANOVAs were used to compare the differences in attentional bias (vigilance/ disengagement) of threatening and sleep-related stimulus among three groups. In stress related physiological activity, CI and HV showed slower recovery rate than LV. Considering attentional bias, HV had
    significant vigilance and disengagement bias to threatening pictures, and CI had significant disengagement bias to sleep-related pictures. CI and HV also showed more prevalent sleep-associated monitoring behaviors than LV. Furthermore, Spearman’s correlation was used to examine the association between attentional bias and pre-sleep arousal. The result shows the attentional bias of HV had negative correlation with reduction of high frequency EEG and somatic sub-score on the PSAS. In contrast to our prediction, CI showed positive correlation between decrease of skin conductance
    and the cognitive sub-score on the PSAS.
    Conclusion
    The study showed that stress recovery ability and stress-related attentional bias were the major differences between individuals with low and high sleep vulnerability,
    indicating that increased information processing to threats and stress-related stimulus as well as decreased recovery ability of autonomic arousal in reacting to stress may
    predisposed an individual to stress-related sleep disturbances. On the other hand, the
    attention shift from threat toward sleep can differentiate chronic insomnia from those individual with frequent acute insomnia. Moreover, the difficulty in disengagement
    from sleep-related stimulus, rather the vigilance, might explain the cause of hyperarousal that perpetuate insomnia. The results support the transition of arousal from general treat to sleep-related stimulus in the development of chronic insomnia. The study not only further the understanding of the etiological mechanism of insomnia, but also imply that different strategies should be applied in the treatment of
    acute and chronic insomnia. It also highlights the importance of preventive intervention for individuals with high sleep vulnerability
    Reference: 車先蕙、盧孟良、陳錫中、張尚文、李宇宙 (2006)。中文版貝克焦慮量表之
    信效度。台灣醫學, 10(4), 447-454。
    林一真 (2000)。貝克焦慮量表 (BAI) 中文版。台北:中國行為科學社。
    林詩淳 (2008)。慢性失眠者與情境性失眠高危險族群之壓力因應與失眠的關
    係。國立政治大學心理所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    林晏瑄 (2011)。睡眠脆弱特質對認知激發狀態下的 睡眠中訊息處理之影響。國立政治大學心理所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    陳心怡 (2000)。貝克憂鬱量表第二版 (BDI-II) 中文版。台北:中國行為科學社。
    翁嘉英、游秀英、周裕軒 (2009)。慢性緊縮型頭痛患者在心理壓力下之顱周肌及自主神經活動特性。臨床心理學刊,4(1), 31-42。
    楊建銘、許世杰、林詩淳、周映妤、陳瑩明 (2009)。失眠嚴重度量表中文版的信、效度探討。臨床心理學刊, 4(2), 95-104。
    詹雅雯、陳昌偉、楊建銘、林詩淳 (2009)。中文版睡前激發狀態量表之信效度探討。臨床心理學刊, 4 (1), 51-58。
    盧孟良、車先蕙、張尚文、沈武典 (2002)。中文版貝克憂鬱量表第二版之信度和效度。台灣精神醫學, 16(4), 301-310。




    Agnew, H. W., Jr., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: An EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology, 2(3), 263-266.
    American Association Sleep Medicine. (2005). International Classification of Sleep Disorders 2nd Ed. (ICSD-2): Diagnostic and coding manual. Westchester, IL.
    American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
    Ansfield, M. E., Wegner, D. M., & Bowser, R. (1996). Ironic effects of sleep urgency. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(7), 523-531.
    Barclay, N. L., & Ellis, J. G. (2013). Sleep-related attentional bias in poor versus good sleepers is independent of affective valence. Journal of Sleep Research, 22(4), 414-421.
    Bastien, C. H., Fortier-Brochu, E., Rioux, I., LeBlanc, M., Daley, M., & Morin, C. M. (2003). Cognitive performance and sleep quality in the elderly suffering from chronic insomnia. Relationship between objective and subjective measures. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54(1), 39-49.
    Bastien, C. H., St-Jean, G., Morin, C. M., Turcotte, I., & Carrier, J. (2008). Chronic psychophysiological insomnia: hyperarousal and/or inhibition deficits? An ERPs investigation. Sleep, 31(6), 887-898.
    Bastien, C. H., St-Jean, G., Turcotte, I., Morin, C. M., Lavallee, M., & Carrier, J. (2009). Sleep spindles in chronic psychophysiological insomnia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 66(1), 59-65.
    Bastien, C. H., Vallieres, A., & Morin, C. M. (2004). Precipitating factors of insomnia. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 2(1), 50-62.
    Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1-24.
    Bélanger, L., Morin, C. M., Gendron, L., & Blais, F. C. (2005). Presleep cognitive activity and thought control strategies in insomnia. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 19(1), 19-28.
    Bonnet, M.H. (1990). The perception of sleep onset in normals and insomniacs. In R. Bootzin, J. Kihlstrom, and D. Schacter (Eds.). Sleep and Cognition. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1992). Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep, 15(6), 526-536.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1995). 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep, 18(7), 581-588.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1997). Hyperarousal and insomnia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 1(2), 97-108.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1998). Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(5), 610-615.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (2000). Activity, arousal, and the MSLT in patients with insomnia. Sleep, 23(2), 205-212.
    Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (2003). Situational Insomnia: Consistency, Predictors, and Outcomes. Sleep, 26(8), 1029-1036.
    Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.
    Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour research and therapy, 25(6), 443-455.
    Brannon, L., & Feist, J. (2000). Health Psychology: An Introduction to Behavior and Health (4th ed). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.
    Broman, J. E., & Hetta, J. (1994). Perceived pre-sleep arousal in patients with persistent psychophysiologic and psychiatric insomnia. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 48(3), 203-207.
    Brosschot, J. F., Dijk, E. van & Thayer, J. F. (2007). Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63(1), 39-47.
    Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60(2), 113-124.
    Burgos, I., Richter, L., Klein, T., Fiebich, B., Feige, B.,…Riemann, D. (2006). Incseased nocturnal interleukin-6 excretion in patients with primary insomnia: A pilot study. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 20(3), 246-253.
    Buysse, D. J., Thompson, W., Scott, J., Franzen, P. L., Germain, A.,….Kupfer, D. J. (2007). Daytime Symptoms in Primary Insomnia: A Prospective Analysis Using Ecological Momentary Assessment. Sleep Medicine, 8(3), 198-208.
    Buysse, D. J., Angst, J., Gamma, A., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D., & Rossler, W. (2008). Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression in young adults. Sleep, 31(4), 473-480.
    Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (2007). Handbook of Psychophysiology, 3rd edition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
    Calvo, M. G., & Avero, P. (2005). Time course of attentional bias to emotional scenes
    in anxiety: Gaze direction and duration. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 433-451.
    Chan, S. F., Chen, T. H., Liao, Y. M., Chou, K. R., & Tsai, P. S. (2011). Development and preliminary validation of the Chinese version of the Sleep-Associated Monitoring Index. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 49(1), 54-64.
    Chen, C. H. (1994). An exploration of postpartum depression model using LISREL. The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 10(5), 229-238.
    Chen, H. C., Lin, C. M., Lee, M. B., & Chou, P. (2011). The Relationship between Pre-sleep Arousal and Spontaneous Arousals from Sleep. Journal of the Chinese Medical Association, 74(2), 84-86.
    Clark, D. M. (1999). Anxiety disorders: Why they persist and how to treat them. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37 (S1), S5-S27.
    Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385-396.
    Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In S. Spacapam & S. Oskamp (Eds.), The social psychology of health: Claremont Symposium on applied social psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Collins, P. Y., Patel, V., Joestl, S. S., March, D., Insel T. R., & Daar A. S. (2011). Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature, 475(7354), 27-30.
    Coren, S., & Mah, K. B. (1993). Prediction of physiological arousability: a validation of the Arousal Predisposition Scale. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 31(2), 215-219.
    Dalgleish, T., & Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 10(5), 589-604.
    DeLongis, A., Coyne, J. C., Dakof, G., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts, and major life events to health status. Health Psychology, 1(2), 119-136.
    DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily stress on health and mood: psychological and social resources as mediators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 486-495.
    De Ruiter, C., & Brosschot, J. F. (1994). The emotional Stroop inteference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(3), 315-319.
    Drake, C. L, Friedman, N. P., Wright, K. P. Jr, & Roth, T. (2011). Sleep reactivity and insomnia: genetic and environmental influences. Sleep, 34(9), 1179-1188.
    Drake, C. L., Jefferson, C., Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (2006). Stress-related sleep disturbance and polysomnographic response to caffeine. Sleep Medicine, 7(7), 567-572.
    Drake, C. L., Richardson, G., Roehrs, T., Scofield, H., & Roth, T. (2004). Vulnerability to stress-related sleep disturbance and hyperarousal. Sleep, 27(2), 285-291.
    Edinger, J. D., Stout, A. L., & Hoelscher, T. J. (1988). Cluster analysis of insomniacs’ MMPI profiles: Relation of subtypes to sleep history and treatment outcome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50(1), 77–87.
    Edinger, J. D., Fins, A. I., Glenn, D. M., Sullivan, R. J., Jr., Bastian, L. A., Marsh, G. R.,…Vasilas, D. (2000). Insomnia and the eye of the beholder: are there clinical markers of objective sleep disturbances among adults with and without insomnia complaints? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 586-593.
    Ellis, J. & Cropley, M. (2002). An examination of thought control strategies employed by acute and chronic insomniacs. Sleep Medicine, 3(5), 393-400.
    Ellis, J. G., Perlis, M. L., Neale, L. F., Espie C. A., & Bastien C. H. (2012). The natural history of insomnia: Focus on prevalence and incidence of acute insomnia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(10), 1278-1285.
    Espie, C. A. (2002). Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 215-243.
    Espie, C. A., Broomfield, N. M., MacMahon, K. M., Macphee, L. M., & Taylor, L. M. (2006). The attention-intention-effort pathway in the development of psychophysiologic insomnia: a theoretical review. Sleep Medicine Review, 10 (4), 215-245.
    Fernández-Mendoza, J., Calhoun, S. L., Bixler, E. O., Karataraki, M., Liao, D.,… Vgontzas, A.N. (2010). Sleep misperception and chronic insomnia in the general population: role of objective sleep duration and psychological profiles. Psychosomatic Medicine, 73(1), 88-97.
    Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend, 97(1-2), 1-20.
    Freedman, R. R. (1986). EEG power spectra in sleep-onset insomnia. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 63(5), 408-413.
    Gross, R. T., & Borkovec, T. D. (1982). Effects of a cognitive intrusion manipulation on the sleep-onset latency of good sleepers. Behavior Therapy, 13(1), 112-116.
    Hall, M., Vasko, R., Buysse, D., Ombao, H., Chen, Q., Cashmere, J. D.,.... Thayer J. F. (2004). Acute stress affects heart rate variability during sleep. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(1), 56-62.
    Han, K. S., Kim, L, & Shim, I. (2012). Stress and sleep disorder. Experimental Neurobiology, 21(4), 141-150.
    Harvey, A. G. (2000). Pre-sleep cognitive activity: a comparison of sleep-onset insomniacs and good sleepers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39(Pt3), 275-286.
    Harvey, A. G. (2002). A cognitive model of insomnia. Behavior Research Therapy, 40(8), 869–893.
    Healey, S. E. , Kales, A., Monroe, L. J., Bixler, E. O., Chamberlin, K., & Soldatos, C. R. (1981). Onset of Insomnia: Role of Life-Stress Events. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43(5), 439-451.
    Hutchinson, K.N., Song, Y., Wang, L., Malow, B.A. (2007). Analysis of sleep parameters in patients with obstructive sleep apnea studied in a hospital vs. a hotel-based sleep center. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 4(2), 119–122.
    Jansson-Fröjmark, M., Bermås, M., & Kjellén A. (2013). Attentional Bias in Insomnia: The Dot-Probe Task with Pictorial Stimuli Depicting Daytime Fatigue/Malaise. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(3), 534-546.
    Jarrin, D. C., Chen, I. Y., Ivers, H., Lamy, M., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2010). Nocturnal heart rate variability in patients treated with cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia. Health Psycholology, 35(6), 638-641.
    Jones, B. T., Macphee, L. J., Broomfield, N. M., Jones, B. C., & Espie, C. A. (2005). Sleep-related attentional bias in good, moderate, and poor (primary insomnia) sleepers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 249-258.
    Kales, A., Caldwell, A. B., Preston, T. A., Healey, S., & Kales, J. D. (1976). Personality patterns in insomnia. Theoretical implications. Archive of Gen Psychiatry, 33(9), 1128-1124.
    Kales, A., Caldwell, A. B., Soldatos, C. R., Bixler, E. O., & Kales, J. D. (1983). Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern specificity and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45(4), 341-356.
    Kao, C. C., Huang, C.J., Wang, M. Y., & Tsai, P. S. (2008). Insomnia: prevalence and its impact on excessive daytime sleepiness and psychological well-being in the adult Taiwanese population. Quality of Life Research, 17(8), 1073-1080.
    Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183-1192.
    Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to Threat in Anxiety-prone Individuals: Mechanisms Underlying Attentional Bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(5), 635-643.
    Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & Van Damme, S. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757-1771.
    Koster, E. H. W., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S. (2005). Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1087-1098.
    Lamarche, C. H., & Ogilvie, R. D. (1997). Electrophysiological changes during the sleep onset period of psychophysiological insomniacs, psychiatric insomniacs, and normal sleepers. Sleep, 20(9), 724-733.
    Lazarus, R., Speisman, J. C., & Mordkoff, A. M. (1963). The relationship between autonomic indicators of psychological stress: heart rate and skin conductance. Psychosomatic Medicine, 25(1), 19-30.
    Le Bon, O., Hoffmann, G., Tecco, J., Staner, L., Noseda, A., Pelc, I., & Linkowski, P. (2000). Mild to moderate sleep respiratory events: one negative night may not be enough. Chest, 118(2), 353–359.
    Le Bon, O., Staner, L., Hoffmann, G., Dramaix, M., San Sebastian, I.,…Linkowski, P. (2001). The first-night effect may last more than one night. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 35(3), 165–172.
    Lichstein, K. L., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1980). Insomniacs` perceptions of cognitive versus somatic determinants of sleep disturbance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89(1), 105-107.
    Lundh, L.G., Fröding, A., Gyllenhammar, L., Broman, J.E., & Hetta, J. (1997). Cognitive bias and memory performance in patients with persistent insomnia. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 26(1), 27-35.
    MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15-20.
    MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107-123.
    MacMahon, K. M. A., Broomfield, N. M., & Espie, C. A. (2006). Attention Bias for Sleep-Related Stimuli in Primary Insomnia and Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome Using the Dot-Probe Task. Sleep, 29(11), 1420-1427.
    Marchetti, L. M., Biello, S. M., Broomfield, N. M., Macmahon, K. M. A., & Espie, C. A. (2006). Who is pre-occupied with sleep? A comparison of attention bias in people with psychophysiological insomnia, delayed sleep phase syndrome and good sleepers using the induced change blindness paradigm. Journal of Sleep Research, 15(2), 212-221.
    Maslow, A.H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4), 370–396.
    Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 16(3), 331-354.
    Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29-45.
    Monroe, L. J. (1967). Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72(3), 255-264.
    Morin, C. M. (1993). Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
    Morin, C. M., Belanger, L., LeBlanc, M., Ivers, H., Savard, J.,…Grégoire J.P.. (2009). The natural history of insomnia: a population-based 3-year longitudinal study. Archives Internal Medicine, 169(5), 447-453.
    Morin, C. M., & Espie, C. A. (1993). Insomnia: psychological assessment and management. New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum.
    Morin, C. M., Rodrigue, S., & Ivers, H. (2003). Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(2), 259-267.
    Musa, C., Lépine, J. P., Clark, D. M., Mansell, W., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Selective attention in social phobia and the moderating effect of concurrent depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(9), 1043-1054.
    Neckelmann, D., Mykletun, A., & Dahl A. A. (2007). Chronic Insomnia as a Risk Factor for Developing Anxiety and Depression. Sleep, 30(7), 873-880.
    Nicassio, P. M., Mendlowitz, D. R., Fussell, J. J., & Petras, L. (1985). The phenomenology of the pre-sleep state: the development of the pre-sleep arousal scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(3), 263-271.
    Nomura, K., Yamaoka, K., Nakao, M., & Yano, E. (2005). Impact of insomnia on individual health dissatisfaction in Japan, South Korea, and Taiwan. Sleep, 28 (10), 1328-1332.
    Ogilvie, R. D., & Harsh, J. R. (1994). Sleep onset: Normal and abnormal processes. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
    Perlis, M. L., Giles, D. E., Mendelson, W. B., Bootzin, R. R., & Wyatt, J. K. (1997). Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioural model and a neurocognitive perspective. Journal of Sleep Research, 6(3), 179-188.
    Perlis, M. L., Smith, M. T., Andrews, P. J., Orff, H., & Giles, D. E. (2001). Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep, 24(1), 110-117.
    Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention systems of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.
    Ree, M. J., & Harvey, A. G. (2004). Investigating Safety Behaviours in Insomnia: The Development of the Sleep-related Behaviours Questionnaire (SRBQ). Behaviour Change, 21(1), 26-36.
    Richardson, C., Gradisar, M., & Pulford, A. (2015). The development of insomnia or the plasticity of good sleep? A preliminary study of acute changes in sleep and insomnia resulting from an analogue trauma. Behavioral sleep medicine, 13(1), 19-35.
    Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews, 18(3), 247-291.
    Robertson, J. A., Broomfield, N. B., & Espie, C. A. (2007). Prospective comparison of subjective arousal during the pre-sleep period in primary sleep-onset insomnia and normal sleepers. Journal of Sleep Research, 16(2), 230-238.
    Roth, T., Roehrs, T., &Vogel, G. (1995). Zolpidem in the treatment of transient insomnia: a double- blind, randomized comparison with placebo. Sleep, 18(4), 246-251.
    Sadeh, A., Hauri, P. J., Kripke, D. F., & Lavie, P. (1995). The role of actigraphy in the evaluation of sleep disorders. Sleep, 18(4), 288-302.
    Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive account. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19(1), 6-19.
    Sarafino, E.P. (2002) . Health psychology: biopsychosocial interactions (4th ed). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
    Schwartz M. S. & Andrasik F. (2016). Biofeedback Fourth Edition: A Practitioner`s Guide. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
    Semler, C. N., & Harvey, A. G. (2004a). An investigation of monitoring for sleep-related threat in primary insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(12), 1403-1420.
    Semler, C. N., & Harvey, A. G. (2004b). Monitoring for sleep-related threat: a pilot study of the Sleep Associated Monitoring Index (SAMI). Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(2), 242-250.
    Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J.,…Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(S20), 22-33.
    Simons, D. J. (2000). Current Approaches to Change Blindness. Visual Cognition, 7 (1/2/3), 1-15.
    Spielman, A., J. (1986). Assessment of insomnia. Clinical Psychology Review, 6(1), 11-25.
    Spielman, A. J., Caruso, L. S., & Glovinsky, P. B. (1987). A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 10(4), 541-553.
    Spiegelhalder, K., Espie, C. A., Nissen, C., & Riemann, D. (2008). Sleep-related attentional bias in patients with primary insomnia compared with sleep experts and healthy controls. Journal of Sleep Research, 17(2), 191-196.
    Spiegelhalder, K., Espie, C. A., & Riemann, D. (2009). Is sleep-related attentional bias due to sleepiness or sleeplessness? Cognition and Emotion, 23(3), 541-550.
    Spiegelhalder, K., Kyle, S.D., Feige, B., Prem, M., Nissen, C., Espie, C.A., & Riemann, D. (2010). The impact of sleep-related attentional bias on polysomnographically measured sleep in primary insomnia. Sleep, 33(1), 107-112.
    Stepanski, E., Glinn, M., Zorick, F., Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (1994). Heart rate changes in chronic insomnia. Stress Medicine, 10(4), 261–266.
    Steptoe, A., Gibson, E. L., Vuononvirta, R., Williams, E. D., Hamer, M., ….Wardle, J. (2007). The effects of tea on psychophysiological stress responsivity and post-stress recovery: a randomised double-blind trial. Psychopharmacology, 190(1), 81-89.
    Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interferences in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662.
    Tang, N. K., Anne Schmidt, D., & Harvey, A. G. (2007). Sleeping with the enemy: clock monitoring in the maintenance of insomnia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(1), 40-55.
    Taylor, S. E. (1999). Health psychology (4th ed.). New York, US: McGraw-Hill.
    Taylor, L. M., Espie, C. A., & White. C. A. (2003). Attentional bias in people with acute versus persistent insomnia secondary to cancer. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 1(4), 200-212.
    Usui, A., Ishizuka, Y., Obinata, I., Okado, T., Fukuzawa, H., & Kanba, S. (1998). Validity of sleep log compared with actigraphic sleep–wake state. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 53(2), 183–184.
    Vgontzas, A. N., Bixler, E. O., Lin, H. M., Prolo, P., Mastorakos, G.,…Chrousos G. P. (2001). Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: clinical implications. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 86(8), 3787–3794.
    Waters, A. M., Craske, M. G., Bergman, R. L., Naliboff, B. D., Negoro, H., & Ornitz, E. M. (2008). Developmental changes in startle reactivity in school-age children at risk for and with actual anxiety disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70, 158–164
    Wicklow, A., & Espie, C. A. (2000). Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to actigraphic measurement of sleep: towards a cognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(7), 679-693.
    Williams, J., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3-24.
    Woods, H., Marchetti, L. M., Biello, S. M., & Espie, C. A. (2009). The clock as a focus of selective attention in those with primary insomnia: an experimental study using a modified Posner paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 231-236.
    Wolynczyk-Gmaj, D., & Szelenberger, W. (2011). Waking EEG in primary insomnia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 71(3), 387-392.
    Yang, C. M., Chou, C. P., Hsiao, F. C. (2011). The association of dysfunctional beliefs about sleep with vulnerability to stress-related sleep disturbance in young adults. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 9(2), 86-91.
    Yang, C. M, Lin, S. C, & Cheng, C. P. (2013). Transient insomnia versus chronic insomnia: a comparison study of sleep-related psychological/behavioral characteristics. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69(10), 1094-1107.
    Yang, C. M., & Lo, H. S. (2007). ERP evidence of enhanced excitatory and reduced inhibitory processes of auditory stimuli during sleep in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep, 30(5), 585-592.
    Yang, C. M., Spielman, A. J., & Glovinsky, P. (2006). Nonpharmacologic strategies in the management of insomnia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29(4), 895-919.
    Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., Koster, E.H.W. (2014). Dynamics of Attentional Bias to Threat in Anxious Adults: Bias towards and/or Away? PLoS ONE, 9(8), e104025. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104025
    Description: 博士
    國立政治大學
    心理學系
    96752503
    Source URI: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0967525031
    Data Type: thesis
    Appears in Collections:[Department of Psychology] Theses

    Files in This Item:

    File SizeFormat
    503101.pdf5880KbAdobe PDF2735View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback