English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 110080/141030 (78%)
Visitors : 46390771      Online Users : 1335
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    政大機構典藏 > 理學院 > 心理學系 > 學位論文 >  Item 140.119/142894
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/142894


    Title: 教育式線索對嬰兒概括化模仿的影響
    The influence of pedagogical cues on infants’ generalized imitation
    Authors: 李岳庭
    Lee, Yueh-Ting
    Contributors: 黃啟泰
    Huang, Chi-Tai
    李岳庭
    Lee, Yueh-Ting
    Keywords: 模仿
    意圖
    概括化
    社會學習
    自然教育學
    Imitation
    Intention
    Generalization
    Social learning
    Natural pedagogy
    Date: 2022
    Issue Date: 2023-01-05 15:19:14 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 自然教育學理論認為人類與生俱有對教育式線索的敏感度,明示溝通不僅指引嬰兒模仿他人意圖傳達的訊息,且有助於嬰兒將學習的動作或知識概括化至團體中其他成員或歸屬於同類的不相同事物。本研究目的在於釐清歸因意圖與溝通互動在概括化模仿可能的角色。我們依循Gergely等人(2002)的光盒派典,他們發現:當大人雙手閒置放在桌上(手自由)示範用頭開燈的動作,比當她雙手抓著毛毯(手限制)示範開燈動作時,更成功引發嬰兒模仿。在五組不同觀察情境中,18個月大的嬰兒(N = 100)觀看實驗者透過眼神和口語或鈴聲,吸引其注意手自由示範的頭開燈動作,其中一半的嬰兒在測試時拿到與示範相同的光盒(溝通示範/相同光盒、意圖示範/相同光盒),另一半則拿到大小顏色不同的光盒(溝通示範/尺寸顏色改變光盒、意圖示範/尺寸顏色改變光盒),參照比較/手限制組則觀看手限制且為溝通互動的方式示範。結果分析顯示各組間的模仿人數有顯著差異,在事後比較發現只有溝通示範/相同光盒組的模仿人數顯著高於參照比較/手限制。由研究發現可知,動作意圖本身並不是引發嬰兒模仿的唯一因素,嬰兒必須依賴溝通線索來決定是否模仿示範的意圖動作,雖然在概括化組別的結果中沒有支持自然教育學理論的預測,但是不能排除模仿的概括化相較於語言的概括化需要更多的認知資源,或是知覺複雜度對概括化模仿的影響。
    The natural pedagogy theory holds that human beings are inherently sensitive to communication cues, and explicit communication not only guides infants to imitate the intended messages of others, but also helps infants generalize learned actions or knowledge to other members of the group or to belong to same kind of different things. The purpose of this study was to clarify the possible roles of attributional intention and communicative interaction in generalized imitation. We followed Gergely et al.’s (2002) light-box paradigm, and they found that infants tended to imitate the action of turning on the light-box with her forehead when an adult demonstrated with her hands resting on the table (hands-free), but not when she was clutching the blanket with both hands (hands-occupied). We designed 5 conditions for 18-month-old infants (N = 100), the first independent variable was the pedagogical cues, whether the experimenter’s action was communicative or intentional to the infant; the other was the generalization, half of the babies were given the same light box as the demonstration during the test, and the other half were given light boxes of different size and color (transferred). The first four conditions are the hands-free condition, the final is hands-occupied condition (control group). Results analysis showed that there were significant differences in the number of imitations among the groups, and post-hoc comparison found that only the number of imitations in the communicative condition was significantly higher than that in the hands-occupied condition. According to the research findings, action intention itself is not the only factor that triggers infants’ imitation. Infants must rely on communicative cues to decide whether to imitate the demonstrated intentional action, which is contrary to the prediction of natural pedagogy theory. However, it cannot be ruled out that the generalization of imitation requires more cognitive resources than the generalization of language, or the influence of perceptual complexity on generalized imitation.
    Reference: Asher, Y. M., & Nelson, D. G. K. (2008). Was it designed to do that? Children’s focus on intended function in their conceptualization of artifacts. Cognition, 106(1), 474-483. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.01.007
    Barnat, S. B., Klein, P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Deferred imitation across changes in context and object: Memory and generalization in 14-month-old infants. Infant behavior and development, 19(2), 241-251. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90023-5
    Barr, R., Dowden, A., & Hayne, H. (1996). Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6-to 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(2), 159-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90015-6
    Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(1), 153-164. https://doi.org/10.1080/027249800390718
    Bruner, J. (1995). From joint attention to the meeting of minds: An introduction. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, 1-14.
    Butler, L. P., Gibbs, H. M., & Levush, K. C. (2020). Look again: Pedagogical demonstration facilitates children’s use of counterevidence. Child Development, 91(6), 1194-1210. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/cdev.13414
    Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2012). Preschoolers use intentional and pedagogical cues to guide inductive inferences and exploration. Child Development, 83(4), 1416-1428. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01775.x
    Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2014). Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition, 130(1), 116-127. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.002
    Butler, L. P., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Two-and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical cues in guiding inductive generalization and exploration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145, 64-78. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.001
    Buttelmann, D., & Zmyj, N. (2012). Evaluating the empirical evidence for the two-stage-model of infant imitation. A commentary on Paulus, Hunnius, Vissers, and Bekkering (2011). Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 512. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00512
    Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2), 315–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1
    Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve‐and 18‐month‐olds copy actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8(1), F13-F20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00385.x
    Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253-278. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1207/s15327078in0803_4
    Chen, M. L., & Waxman, S. R. (2013). “Shall we blick?”: Novel words highlight actors` underlying intentions for 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 426-431. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0029486
    Cimpian, A., & Markman, E. M. (2005). The absence of a shape bias in children`s word learning. Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 1003. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.1003
    Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016). Instrumental and conventional interpretations of behavior are associated with distinct outcomes in early childhood. Child Development, 87(2), 527-542. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12472
    Collie, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Deferred imitation by 6‐and 9‐month‐old infants: More evidence for declarative memory. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 35(2), 83-90. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199909)35:2%3C83::aid-dev1%3E3.0.co;2-s
    Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and performance XXI, 21, 249-274.
    Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755-755. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1038/415755a
    Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction, 229-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003135517-11
    Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165-193. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-h
    Goldberg, A. E. (2009). The nature of generalization in language. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(1), 93–127. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1515/COGL.2009.005
    Grossmann, T., Johnson, M. H., Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Deligianni, F., Elwell, C., & Csibra, G. (2008). Early cortical specialization for face-to-face communication in human infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1653), 2803-2811. https://dx.doi.org/10.1098%2Frspb.2008.0986
    Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V., & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7‐month‐old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science, 11(4), 487-494. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x
    Hauf, P., & Aschersleben, G. (2008). Action–effect anticipation in infant action control. Psychological Research, 72(2), 203-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0101-3
    Király, I., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2013). Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of experimental child psychology, 116(2), 471-486. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003
    Király, I., Oláh, K., Csibra, G., & Kovács, Á. M. (2018). Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11477-11482. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1803505115
    Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive development, 3(3), 299-321. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/0885-2014(88)90014-7
    Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688-699. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005
    Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142, 351-361. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.020
    Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science, 12(2), 264-271. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00758.x
    Marno, H., & Csibra, G. (2015). Toddlers favor communicatively presented information over statistical reliability in learning about artifacts. PloS one, 10(3), e0122129. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122129
    Mejía-Arauz, R., Rogoff, B., & Paradise, R. (2005). Cultural variation in children`s observation during a demonstration. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(4), 282-291. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1177/01650250544000062
    Meltzoff, A. N. (1988a). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 470. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.470
    Meltzoff, A. N. (1988b). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59(1), 217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03210.x
    Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838-850. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.31.5.838
    Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75-78. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.897687
    Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54(3), 702-709. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2307/1130058
    Nelson, D. G. K., Russell, R., Duke, N., & Jones, K. (2000). Two-year-olds will name artifacts by their functions. Child development, 71(5), 1271-1288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00228
    Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1050-1053. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0023866
    Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children`s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182-192. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024555
    Paulus, M. (2014). The emergence of prosocial behavior: Why do infants and toddlers help, comfort, and share? Child Development Perspectives, 8(2), 77-81. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12066
    Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Vissers, M., & Bekkering, H. (2011a). Bridging the gap between the other and me: The functional role of motor resonance and action effects in infants’ imitation. Developmental Science, 14(4), 901-910. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01040.x
    Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Vissers, M., & Bekkering, H. (2011b). Imitation in infancy: Rational or motor resonance? Child Development, 82(4), 1047-1057. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01610.x
    Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans.). Basic Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/11168-000
    Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development . Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253(5489), 265-266. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1038/253265a0
    Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668-671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059
    Shneidman, L., Gaskins, S., & Woodward, A. (2016). Child‐directed teaching and social learning at 18 months of age: evidence from Yucatec Mayan and US infants. Developmental Science, 19(3), 372-381. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/desc.12318
    Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual review of anthropology, 28(1), 509-529. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.509
    Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121-125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x
    Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.906
    Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. In Mind the gap (pp. 331-349). Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-02725-3_16
    Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erdohegyi, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Infants` perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science, 321(5897), 1831-1834. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1161437
    Whiten, A., Custance, D. M., Gomez, J.-C., Teixidor, P., & Bard, K. A. (1996). Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(1), 3-14. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0735-7036.110.1.3
    Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers` understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 723-731. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.723
    Wohlschläger, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 501-515. https://dx.doi.org/10.1098%2Frstb.2002.1257
    Yoon, J. M., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2008). Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(36), 13690-13695. https://dx.doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0804388105
    Description: 碩士
    國立政治大學
    心理學系
    108752002
    Source URI: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0108752002
    Data Type: thesis
    DOI: 10.6814/NCCU202201737
    Appears in Collections:[心理學系] 學位論文

    Files in This Item:

    File Description SizeFormat
    200201.pdf1608KbAdobe PDF20View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback