English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 110934/141854 (78%)
Visitors : 47816646      Online Users : 602
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/55736


    Title: 漢語親子對話中的第二人稱指涉詞
    Other Titles: Second Person Reference in Mandarin Parent-Child Conversation
    Authors: 黃瓊之
    Contributors: 國立政治大學語言學研究所
    行政院國家科學委員會
    Keywords: 指涉詞選擇;第二人稱指涉詞;語言習得
    referential choice;second person reference;language acquisition
    Date: 2009
    Issue Date: 2012-11-21 10:35:33 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 漢語有省略論元的特徵。在漢語中若指涉對象可從語境中瞭解,外顯指涉詞通常可以省略(Huang, 1994, 2000; Li & Thompson, 1981, Wang et al., 1992)。也就是說,漢語說話者指涉詞的選擇有言談上的動機。過去的研究顯示成人說話者對使用不同指涉詞的言談語用因素是敏感的(Chafe, 1994; Du Bois, 1985, 1987; Givon, 1983; Gundel, Hedberg & Zacharski, 1993; Kumpf, 1992)。在語言習得研究中,語法和言談常常被當作是分開的、沒有任何顯著互動的領域。由於以言談語用的方式來解釋成人語言中指涉詞的使用已獲的很好的研究成果,最近有一些研究也用此方式來研究幼童指涉詞的使用。研究顯示幼童的指涉詞選擇也受語用原則的影響(Allen, 2000; Clancy, 1993; 1997; Guerriero, Oshima-Takane & Kuriyama, 2006; Narasimhan, Budwig & Murty, 2005; Serratrice, 2005)。如果漢語可以省略指涉詞,一個有趣的問題是:為什麼有時候當指涉對象清楚明瞭時幼童和其父母在對話中仍會使用外顯指涉詞?通常指涉說話者及聽話者的指涉詞是最易被省略的,因為被省略的指涉詞很容易能從互動語境中瞭解。由於語境就能使內隱的第二人稱指涉詞明確化,因此本研究的目的是要探討促使幼童和其父母使用外顯第二人稱指涉詞的因素。從語用的觀點,本研究將分析幼童的第二人稱指涉詞是否與特定的溝通意圖或語用功能有關。同時如Guerriero et al. (2006) 所指出,父母的語言輸入也可能影響幼童指涉詞的使用。因此本研究也將分析父母言談中第二人稱指涉詞的使用情形。
    It is well-known that Mandarin permits omitted arguments. In Mandarin, overt reference forms can be omitted provided that the referent can be understood from the context (Huang, 1994, 2000; Li & Thompson, 1981, Wang et al., 1992). In other words, Mandarin speakers’ referential choices may be discourse-motivated. Previous studies have indicated that adult speakers show sensitivity to discourse-pragmatic factors presumed to underlie the differential use of referring expressions in discourse (Chafe, 1994; Du Bois, 1985, 1987; Givon, 1983; Gundel, Hedberg & Zacharski, 1993; Kumpf, 1992). In language acquisition research, grammar and discourse are frequently treated as separate domains that do not interact in any significant way. Given the success of the discourse-pragmatic approach in explaining the distribution of referring expressions in adult language, recently a few studies have investigated the child’s referential choice from this use-oriented perspective. It has been reported that the child’s referential choice can also be explained by pragmatic principles (Allen, 2000; Clancy, 1993; 1997; Guerriero, Oshima-Takane & Kuriyama, 2006; Narasimhan, Budwig & Murty, 2005; Serratrice, 2005). If Mandarin permits omitted reference forms, an interesting question would be why Mandarin-speaking children and parents sometimes do supply overt reference when the referent is understood. It is commonly argued that reference forms for referring to the speaker and the addressee are most readily omitted because they are easily retrievable from the physical interactional context (Oh, 2007). Given the availability of discourse context to disambiguate implicit reference to the addressee, the purpose of this study is to investigate what might provide the impetus for Mandarin-speaking children and their parents to attempt overt reference to the addressee. From a pragmatic perspective, we attempt to investigate whether children’s second person reference forms are associated with particular communicative intents or pragmatic functions. Furthermore, as suggested by Guerriero, et al. (2006), parental input may also influence children’s referential choice. Thus, the patterns of the parents’ use of second person reference will also be examined.
    Relation: 基礎研究
    學術補助
    研究期間:9808~ 9907
    研究經費:663仟元
    Data Type: report
    Appears in Collections:[語言學研究所] 國科會研究計畫

    Files in This Item:

    File SizeFormat
    982410H004099.pdf406KbAdobe PDF2733View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback